Using Examples to Promote Learning

Credit: iStock.com/SinArtCreative
Credit: iStock.com/SinArtCreative

Teachers employ a vast array of instructional methods, but one universal element is the use of examples. No teaching approach eschews examples. On the contrary, guides for effective teaching embrace the value of using good examples (e.g., Rosenshine, 2012). Given their importance, teachers should design and use examples carefully and assess their impact on student learning. Little guidance, however, exists to help teachers accomplish these tasks. In this article, I synthesize the research on examples in the hopes of providing that guidance.

Defining examples

I define an example as “any specific instance, illustration, demonstration or activity that is representative of a concept” (Chew, 2007, p. 74). This intentionally broad definition covers exemplars that illustrate concepts, as well as analogies, stories, learning activities, worked problems, and problem sets. We typically think of examples as prototypes for a concept, such as Bach as an example of Baroque music, but they can take other forms, such as metaphors (e.g., the heart is like a pump). Teaching activities designed get students to think about a concept can be examples. In a literature course, students may learn about foreshadowing by identifying examples in a literary work. Worked problems and problems sets can also serve as examples of a concept, especially in STEM disciplines.

How examples are supposed to work

If we understand what examples contribute to learning, we can use them more effectively and diagnose the problems when examples fail. Learning from examples is a complex process with errors possible at multiple points (Chew, 2007). First, the teacher presents a new, more advanced concept, which students often find abstract and vague. Then the teacher presents an example, which should ground the abstract concept in a concrete reality that students can grasp. Now students have to generalize from the example back to the abstract concept. They need to map the elements of the example onto the characteristics of the concept such that they can now recognize and analyze new examples of the concept. For instance, in my introductory psychology class, I teach the concept of conformity, a form of social influence. Conformity, as I define it, is when people change their behavior to match the behavior of others. After presenting the definition, I might give this example: You start a new job and you notice that the other employees wear a certain brand of polo shirt. You hurry out and buy those shirts. Ideally, when students map relevant parts of the example onto the definition of conformity, their understanding deepens, and they start to recognize other examples of conformity.

How might an example fail? If the example is too complex, ambiguous, or confusing, it will fail. Good examples are simpler than the concepts they represent. Unfortunately, the curse of expertise works against teachers’ abilities to judge the effectiveness of explanations and examples. Experts typically underestimate how quickly and easily novices will learn concepts (Fisher & Keil, 2016). Next, students may confuse knowing the example with understanding the concept. Chi (Chi et al., 1989; Chiu & Chi, 2014) found that higher-achieving students explain to themselves how examples relate to concepts without prompting. Struggling students, however, tend to simply record the example without self-explanation or reflection. Finally, students may never generalize from the concrete example to a general, abstract understanding. They may think that conformity relates only to workplace clothing decisions.

Most examples break into two parts: the surface component and the structural component (Chew, 2007). The surface component refers to the contextual parts of the example that are not relevant to its representation of the concept. These include such elements as storyline, wording, objects, and numbers that can be changed without altering the nature of the example. In my conformity example, the surface components include the workplace setting and polo shirts. The example could describe a school setting with brands of shoes and still be about conformity. The structural component embodies the essential elements of the concept. Changing the structural component changes the example. If the manager tells all new employees to wear polo shirts, the example is no longer about conformity but about obedience. Typically, the surface component is more familiar to students than the structural component, and students may confuse the surface structure with the concept. Here are some examples of conformity with different surface components but the same structural component.

  • You attend your first orchestra concert. The concertmaster comes onto the stage and the audience applauds. You applaud too, but you aren’t sure why.
  • A college student attends a Catholic Mass for the first time with a friend. She is sitting in the pew when suddenly everyone kneels down. She kneels down as well.
  • A high school student is hanging out with his friends late one night. The group decides it would be fun to spray-paint lewd drawings on the wall of the school. The student feels like he has to go along and take part.

The surface component determines the level of student familiarity and interest. The more familiar and interesting the example is to students, the more likely its effectiveness for learning. Teachers need to select and adapt examples cognizant of the interests of the students they teach.

Factors to consider in designing examples

What must teachers keep in mind when designing or selecting examples? The following factors merit consideration (Chew, 2007).

  • Student engagement: Good examples are interesting and intriguing. They capture student interest and arouse curiosity. They introduce a new way of understanding issues that students care about.
  • Cognitive load: Cognitive load for examples refers to the amount of mental effort or concentration needed to comprehend an example. Complex, abstract examples will have a higher cognitive load. Students will have a harder time understanding them and be less likely to learn from them (Sweller et al., 2019). Examples should have a lower cognitive load than the concepts they illustrate. That requires simplifying the example while still allowing students to be able to see how the example represents the concept. A good example balances ease of comprehension and cognitive load. But simple examples aren’t always possible. In a literature course, an example might be an entire literary work. Students need time and scaffolding to comprehend complex examples. When selecting examples teachers should remember that oversimplification and overcomplication both harm learning.
  • Familiarity: Generally, familiar examples interest students, but unfamiliar examples can be intriguing. Familiar examples reduce cognitive load because students can use prior knowledge to aid their understanding of the example.
  • Affective response: An emotional response to examples can make them more engaging and memorable, so long as the reaction isn’t strongly negative. My first day on a job example might evoke emotional memories of students’ own experiences starting a new job. Examples that evoke surprise by providing unexpected insights about important issues reinforce learning.
  • Self-explanation and reflection: Chi and Chiu (2014) reviewed decades of research that showed the importance of student self-explanation in learning from examples. Examples that surprise or seem counterintuitive invite self-explanation and reflection. Instead of simply presenting examples, teachers can encourage students to think and talk about them with follow-up questions and other methods that promote reflection and explanation (Lee & Hutchison, 1998).
  • Transfer and application: Examples can promote appropriate transfer and application of course concepts to situations beyond the classroom. The example of high school students vandalizing their school can cause students think about episodes from their own experience where conformity caused people in groups to do things they would not do as individuals.

Using examples effectively

Typically, teachers explain a concept and then provide one or two examples. The rest is up to the student. Research has uncovered ways to increase the effectiveness of examples. For example, teachers can use questions as part of formative assessments to induce student self-reflection on examples (Lee & Hutchinson, 1998). As part of a review, a teacher could provide a novel example of conformity and have students in pairs determine what concept it represents. Students can classify collections of examples of related concepts, such as conformity, compliance, and obedience. Teachers can present a variety of examples with different surface components, such as the ones for conformity, to teach students to differentiate the structural components from the surface components, which improves learning (Butler et al., 2017; Paas & van Merriënboer, 1994).

For STEM classes, teachers can scaffold a series of problems that go from simple to complex. If the teacher works an example problem, then students can first solve a problem with similar surface components, then a problem with somewhat different surface components, and finally one with markedly different surface components (Hampton & Chew, 2010). Another way of scaffolding learning “fades” examples (Atkinson et al., 2003). The teacher first provides a worked example for students to study and follows with an example that is almost solved, but students have to complete the last step. Students solve a sequence of examples with less and less of the problem solved for them. For other ways to use worked examples, see Paas and van Gog (2006), Renkl (2014), and van Gog et al. (2010).

Designing examples to address cognitive challenges in learning

Examples should be designed and used with a learning goal in mind. Chew and Cerbin (2020) described nine cognitive challenges that teachers must address successfully for students to learn. Examples can be designed to address at least four of those challenges: student mental mindset, metacognition, ineffective learning strategies, and transfer. Examples can change student mindsets by showing the relevance and importance of course concepts. The examples of conformity aim to show students the relevance of conformity to their everyday experiences. Hopefully, that changes attitudes about the importance of course content. Metacognitive awareness results from the feedback examples provide about students’ level of understanding of a concept. If an example doesn’t make sense, then the student doesn’t fully understand the concept. Teachers can use examples in ways that promote student learning and model effective learning strategies (Weinstein et al., 2019). Examples can be used for retrieval practice. Teachers can mix up the order of examples and space them out over several days. All these strategies improve learning. Finally, examples powerfully show the applicability of concepts beyond the classroom and give students practice in applying them in novel situations.

Teachers understand the value of good examples for student learning but may not be designing and using examples optimally. This essay provides a research-based framework for designing and using examples that can help teachers maximize the impact of examples on learning.

References

Butler, A. C., Black-Maier, A. C., Raley, N. D., & Marsh, E. J. (2017). Retrieving and applying knowledge to different examples promotes transfer of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 23(4), 433–446. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000142

Chew, S. L. (2007). Designing effective examples and problems for teaching statistics. In D. S. Dunn, R. A. Smith, & B. Beins (Eds.), Best practices for teaching statistics and research methods in the behavioral sciences (pp. 73–91). Erlbaum.

Chew, S. L., & Cerbin, W. J. (2020). The cognitive challenges of effective teaching. The Journal of Economic Educationhttps://doi.org/10.1080/00220485.2020.1845266

Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis. M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanation: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13(2), 145–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/0364-0213(89)90002-5

Chiu, J. L., & Chi, M. T. H. (2014). Supporting self-explanation in the classroom. In V. A. Benassi, C. E. Overson, & C. M. Hakala (Eds.), Applying science of learning in education: Infusing psychological science in the curriculum (pp. 91–103). Society for the Teaching of Psychology. https://teachpsych.org/ebooks/asle2014/index.php

Fisher, M., & Keil, F. C. (2016). The curse of expertise: When more knowledge leads to miscalibrated explanatory insight. Cognitive science, 40(5), 1251–1269. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12280

Hampton, A. G., & Chew, S. L. (2010, January). Designed sequences of examples facilitate learning of statistical concepts [Poster presentation]. National Institute for the Teaching of Psychology, St. Pete Beach, FL.

Lee, A. Y., & Hutchison, L. (1998). Improving learning from examples through reflection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4(3), 187–210. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.4.3.187

Paas, F., & van Gog, T. (2006). Optimising worked example instruction: Different ways to increase germane cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 16(2), 87–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.02.004

Paas, F. G. W. C., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 122–133. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.1.122

Renkl, A. (2014). Learning from worked examples: How to prepare students for meaningful problem solving. In V. A. Benassi, C. E. Overson, & C. M. Hakala (Eds.), Applying science of learning in education: Infusing psychological science into the curriculum (pp. 118–130). Society for the Teaching of Psychology. https://teachpsych.org/ebooks/asle2014/index.php

Rosenshine, B. (2012). Principles of instruction: Research-based strategies that all teachers should know. American Educator, 36(1), 12–19, 39. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ971753.pdf

Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J.J.G. & Paas, F. (2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 261–292. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09465-5

Weinstein, Y., Sumeracki, M., & Caviglioli, O. (2019). Understanding how we learn: A visual guide. Routledge.


Stephen L. Chew, PhD, is a professor of psychology at Samford University. Trained as a cognitive psychologist, his primary research area is the cognitive basis of effective teaching and learning. He is the creator of a groundbreaking series of YouTube videos for students on how to study effectively in college, which have been viewed over three million times and are in wide use from high schools to professional schools. Author contact: slchew@samford.edu.

Leave a Reply

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

Related Articles

I have two loves: teaching and learning. Although I love them for different reasons, I’ve been passionate about...
There are a myriad of answers to this seemingly simple question. Grading probably comes to mind for many...
Fears of disingenuous work, fraudulent and stolen information, and theft of intellectual property have been swirling around education...
Students often struggle academically due to an inability to organize their lives around achievable goals. Students beyond early...
Hey, you. Yes, you. When was the last time you told your students, colleagues, or (gulp) administrators how...

For online faculty, I think it’s more important than ever to be conspicuously human...

Back when I was an undergraduate, students were thought to drop out of college because either they failed...

Are you signed up for free weekly Teaching Professor updates?

You'll get notified of the newest articles.

The Teaching Professor Conference 2024

June 7-9, 2024 • New Orleans

Connect with Fellow Educators at The Teaching Professor Conference!